Fine Art

Cebus apella

Cebus apella (*)

Superregnum: Eukaryota
Cladus: Opisthokonta
Regnum: Animalia
Subregnum: Eumetazoa
Cladus: Bilateria
Cladus: Nephrozoa
Cladus: Deuterostomia
Phylum: Chordata
Subphylum: Vertebrata
Infraphylum: Gnathostomata
Superclassis: Tetrapoda
Classis: Mammalia
Subclassis: Theria
Infraclassis: Placentalia
Ordo: Primates
Subordo: Haplorrhini
Infraordo: Simiiformes
Parvordo: Platyrrhini
Familia: Cebidae
Subfamilia: Cebinae
Genus: Cebus
Species: Cebus apella
Subspecies: C. a. apella - C. a. fatuellus - C. a. macrocephalus - C. a. margaritae - C. a. peruanus - C. a. tocantinus

Name

Cebus apella, (Linnaeus, 1758)

Vernacular names
Internationalization
English: Tufted Capuchin
日本語: フサオマキザル
Português: Macaco-prego
Русский: Капуцин бурый

The Tufted Capuchin (Cebus apella), also known as Brown Capuchin or Black-capped Capuchin is a New World primate from South America. As traditionally defined, it is one of the most widespread primates in the Neotropics, but it has recently been recommended considering the Black-striped, Black and Golden-bellied Capuchins as separate species, thereby effectively limiting the Tufted Capuchin to the Amazon Basin and nearby regions.[1]

The Tufted Capuchin is an omnivorous animal, mostly feeding on fruits and invertebrates, although it sometimes feeds on small vertebrates (e.g. lizards and bird chicks) and other plant parts. It can be found in many different kinds of environment, including moist tropical and subtropical forest, dry forest, and disturbed or secondary forest.

Like other capuchins, it is a social animal, forming groups of 8 to 15 individuals that are led by an alpha or dominant male.

Taxonomy and phylogeny

Previously all tufted capuchins were classified as Cebus apella.[3][4] More recent taxonomic studies have been carried out by Torres de Assumpção (1983; Torres 1988).[5] Groves (2001, 2005) recognized the following subspecies for the tufted capuchins: C. apella apella (Linnaeus, 1758); C. apella fatuellus (Linnaeus, 1766); C. apella macrocephalus Spix, 1823; C. apella peruanus Thomas, 1901; C. apella tocantinus Lönnberg, 1939; C. apella margaritae Hollister, 1914; C. libidinosus libidinosus Spix, 1823; C. libidinosus pallidus Gray, 1866; C. libidinosus paraguayanus Fischer, 1829; C. libidinosus juruanus Lönnberg, 1939; C. nigritus nigritus (Goldfuss, 1809); C. nigritus robustus Kuhl, 1820; C. nigritus cucullatus Spix, 1823; C. xanthosternos Wied-Neuwied, 1826 (see Fragaszy et al. 2004; Rylands et al. 2005).

Physical characteristics

The Tufted Capuchin is more powerfully built than the other capuchins, with rougher fur and a short, thick tail. It has a bundle of long, hardened hair on the forehead that can be raised as a sort of "wig". The fur is brownish gray, with the belly being somewhat lighter-colored than the rest of the body. The hands and feet are black. The tail is strong and can be used as a grasping tail.

The Tufted Capuchin has a head-body length of 32 to 57 centimetres (13 to 22 in), a tail length of 38 to 56 centimetres (15 to 22 in), and a weight of 1.9 to 4.8 kilograms (4.2 to 11 lb), with the males generally being larger and heavier than the females.
[edit] Natural history

The Tufted Capuchin is a diurnal, arboreal primate species, but it often forages on the ground to search for food or to walk longer distances between trees that are too far apart to jump.
Male Tufted Capuchin

The Tufted Capuchin lives in groups of two to twenty or more animals. A single group usually contains at least one adult male, but mixed groups with multiple males do also occur. In that case, one of the males is dominant. He accepts only a few monkeys in his direct surroundings, mainly younger animals and a few females. The dominant male and the group members that are close to him have the privilege to eat first in case of food scarcity, while subordinate monkeys have to wait until they are ready.

After a gestation period of 180 days, one young is born, or incidentally a twin. This young, which weighs only 200 to 250 grams, is carried on the back of its mother. The mother feeds her child for 9 months, but the young is sexually immature until its seventh year, which is quite late for a primate of its size.

Important natural enemies of the capuchin are large birds of prey. They are so afraid of those birds, that they even become alarmed when a harmless bird flies over.

The Tufted Capuchin rubs urine on its hands and feet in order to attract mates and reduce stress.[6]

Diet

A recently discovered characteristic of one population this species is that it uses stones as a tool to open hard nuts. First it chooses ripe nuts from a nut palm. It uses its teeth to strip off the nut's fibrous husk. Then it leaves the nut to dry for about a week. When the nut is dry, the monkey lays the nut on a large, flat stone or fallen tree,[7] after which it hammers with a smaller stone until the nut cracks. The chosen hammer stone is harder than the anvil rock.[8][9]

Besides nuts, the capuchin also eats fruit, insects, larvae, eggs, young birds, frogs, lizards, and even bats. They are also known to chase cats.

The Tufted Capuchin looks for its food in groups. As soon as one of the group members has found something edible, he or she may make a large whistling sound, dependent upon the proximity of other individuals and abundance of the food resource, so that the other monkeys know that there is something to eat.[10] The composition of the group is very well organized, and is determined by rank in the hierarchy. The dominant male often resides somewhere in the middle of the group just behind the front line, so that it is safer when a predator attacks. The vanguard is composed of higher-ranked females who are tolerated by the dominant male. They have the privilege to reach the food first, but they are also the most vulnerable when a predator attacks.
[edit] Tool use and manufacture

The Tufted Capuchin has been observed using containers to hold water, using sticks (to dig nuts, to dip for syrup, to catch ants, to reach food), using stones to hit nuts, using sponges to absorb juice, using stones as hammer and chisel to penetrate a barrier[11] and using stones as hammer and anvil to crack nuts.[12] While some of these tasks are relatively simple by cognitive standards (e.g. using a stick to catch ants), others, like cracking nuts with hammer and anvil are only exceeded in complexity by chimpanzees.[13]

The potential for tool use in animals like the Tufted Capuchin depends on a number of conditions that would increase its likelihood of appearing in a given species. Van Schaik proposed that the occurrence of tool use would be likely in foraging species if three factors were present: manual dexterity, intelligence, and social tolerance.[14] As it applies to manual dexterity, capuchins are capable of a limited precision grip (the ability to delicately pinch and manipulate objects with the thumb and fingertips), which is not found in any other New World monkeys and only found in limited amounts in apes.[15][16] C. apella has an encephalization ratio greater than the hominids (except humans) and a neocortex ratio that is almost as large at the apes; both of these rough indicators suggest high intelligence.[17][18] Finally, the Tufted Capuchin forms social groups typical of a complex and tolerant society.[19][20]

The Tufted Capuchin has been observed manufacturing tools both in captivity and in the wild. In captivity, it has been reported as making probing sticks to reach normally inaccessible containers with syrup.[21] It is also capable of understanding the concept of "sponging" and using paper towels, monkey biscuits, sticks, leaves and straw to sop up juice and then suck on the sponge to consume the juice.[21] Research in the wild has shown that capuchin tool use is every bit as extensive as in captivity with capuchins being observed using stones to dig holes to get at tubers, an activity previously only seen in humans.[22] The practice of using stones to crack nuts has arisen spontaneously in many locations such as in the Caatinga Dry Forest[22] and Serra da Capivara National Park,[23] all in Brazil and hundreds of miles apart. It has been observed cracking various nuts and fruits such as palm nuts (Attalea and Astrocaryum spp.)[24] and jatobá fruits.(Hymenaea courbaril)[23] The Tufted Capuchin has even been observed using stones to dislodge other stones that would later be used as hammers or shovels, an example of a more complex tool using behavior known as second-order tool use previously only found in chimpanzees.[23] Curiously, not all Tufted Capuchins engage in tool use. Moura and Lee (2004)[25] suggest lack of other food sources as the key factor. Ottoni and Mannu (2001),[26] Fragaszy et al. (2004)[27] and Visalberghi et al. (2005)[28] have proposed this is likely more a factor of a monkey's terrestrial habit: the more time a monkey spends on the ground, the more likely it is to profit from (and thus engage in) tool use.

In captivity, the Tufted Capuchin has been seen to manufacture stone tools that produced simple flakes and cores. Some of the capuchins even used these sharpened stones to cut (in a back-and-forth motion) barriers in order to reach food.[11] The importance of this behavior is that it serves as evidence of mechanical proclivity to modify stones by using behaviors already in the monkeys' repertoires, and this behavior is seen as a precursor to stone-knapping.[15] This early and limited tool use behavior has been hypothesized as similar to pre-Homo habilis and that artifacts of that time would probably resemble those of capuchins.[11]

C. apella tool manufacture and use has been analyzed for potential clues to social learning and problem solving ability, as tool manufacture and use can often shed light on such complex cognitive abilities.[11][29] Social learning, or the ability to learn from other individuals, is a controversial topic in most nonhuman species like C. apella because of the relative difficulty of determining whether a behavior was learned from imitation or a much simpler form of social learning.[30] One way of closing the gap between concurrent tool related behaviors and their likelihood of arising from imitation is by narrowing down events that would make social learning more probable such as a preference for observing experienced tool users. In this regard, Ottoni and his team found that young capuchins tended to observe the best tool users when cracking nuts.[12]


Another way of isolating imitation from other simpler behaviors is to present the capuchins with a box that has food but has two different ways of opening it. The important point is that neither way should be more advantageous so that the monkey can freely choose one. In one such study, when humans opened the door in front of the monkeys using one way only, the monkeys used that method, even when they discovered the alternative on their own.[31] In another study, capuchin alphas from two separate groups were trained to open the door in a specific way, after which the monkeys were paired with subordinates who learned to open the door in the same way.[32] When capuchins are trained in the same way and this time released into their groups, the habit is once again disseminated amongst all group members even when others discover alternative ways.[33] Nevertheless, the subject of whether or not C. apella learns by imitation is still controversial, because of the inherent difficulty in teasing out unambiguous evidence of a complex cognitive process such as imitation.[31][34]

Problem solving

Tool use and manufacture can also shed light on the many aspects of the Tufted Capuchin's cognitive abilities by determining how it solves some problems. Some non-primates manufacture and use objects as tools. Crows are known to make hook-tools for catching insects,[35] but such activities lack the behavioral plasticity of tool use as evidenced in tufted capuchins who found new ways to use tools that other species could not.[36] But this plasticity in tool use, while suggesting greater complexity and cognitive ability, does not suggest that the monkeys understand cause and effect. It instead implies they are only able to learn from successful efforts but not from failures nor are they able to refine and improve much.[37] Its ability to repeat successes, coupled with its complex repertoire of behavioral events helps to explain the Tufted Capuchin's extensive repertoire of innovative behaviors besides tool use.[37]

Geographical distribution and habitat

This species lives in the northern Amazon rainforest of the Guyanas, Venezuela and Brazil and to the west of the Rio Negro, as far north as the Orinoco in Venezuela. It is also found in eastern Colombia, Ecuador, Peru, including the upper Andean Magdalena valley in Colombia. It can be found in a large variety of forest types, mainly in tropical rainforests (up till a height of 2700 m), but also in more open forests.

The distribution overlaps with that of other species of capuchins, such as the White-fronted Capuchin (Cebus albifrons).

References

1. ^ a b Groves, C. (2005). Wilson, D. E., & Reeder, D. M, eds. ed. Mammal Species of the World (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. pp. 137. ISBN 0-801-88221-4. OCLC 62265494. http://www.bucknell.edu/msw3/browse.asp?id=12100263.
2. ^ Rylands, A. B., Boubli, J.-P., Mittermeier, R. A. & Wallace, R. B. (2008). Cebus apella. In: IUCN 2008. IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Downloaded on 2 January 2009.
3. ^ Hershkovitz, Philip (1949). "Mammals of northern Colombia. Preliminary report No. 4: Monkeys (primates), with taxonomic revisions of some forms.". Proceedings of the United States National Museum 98:323 pp.: 1–323.
4. ^ Hershkovitz, Phillip (1955). "Notes on American monkeys of the genus Cebus.". Journal of Mammalogy 36: 449–452.
5. ^ Torres, Marguerite; Fragaszy, D.M., Izar, P., Hinley, A.J., Ottoni, E.B., Visalberghi, E. (2007). "Modelling the monkey habitat in Brazil". Geospatial Today (5): 26–35. http://www.istc.cnr.it/doc/28a_20070418142228t_Maddenetal2007-capuchin_habitat_modelling.pdf.
6. ^ "Randy monkeys wash hands, feet in urine. Scientists believe waste-bathing might also calm the primates". 2007-09-25. http://www.msnbc.msn.com/id/20975879/. Retrieved 2008-01-04.
7. ^ Madden, M.; Fragaszy, D.M., Izar, P., Hinley, A.J., Ottoni, E.B., Visalberghi, E. (2007). "Modelling the monkey habitat in Brazil". Geospatial Today (5): 26–35. http://www.istc.cnr.it/doc/28a_20070418142228t_Maddenetal2007-capuchin_habitat_modelling.pdf.
8. ^ Discovery "Life" series, "Challenges of Life" episode, David Attenborough
9. ^ Waga, IC; Dacier, A.K.; Pinha, P.S.; Tavares, M.C.H. (2005). "Spontaneous Tool Use by Wild Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) in the Cerrado". Folia Primatologica 77: 337–344. doi:10.1159/000093698. http://content.karger.com/produktedb/produkte.asp?typ=pdf&file=FPR2006077005337.
10. ^ Di Bitetti, Mario S. (2005). "Food-associated calls and audience effects in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella nigritus". Animal Behavior 69: p. 911–919. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.021.
11. ^ a b c d Westergaard, Gregory Charles (February 1995). "The subsistence technology of capuchins". International Journal of Primatology 16 (1): 899–906. doi:10.1007/BF02736075. http://www.springerlink.com/content/ak635h723h141888/. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
12. ^ a b Ottoni, Eduardo; Briseida Dogo de Resende, Patrícia Izar (2005). "Watching the best nutcrackers: what capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know about others' tool-using skills". Animal Cognition 8 (4): 215–219. doi:10.1007/s10071-004-0245-8. PMID 15719240. http://www.springerlink.com/content/2f1mc842n1et69p0/. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
13. ^ Westergaard, Gregory Charles (1999). "Structural analysis of tool-use by tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)". Animal Cognition 2 (3): 141–145. doi:10.1007/s100710050034. http://www.springerlink.com/content/e8rc17jwkty95avp/. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
14. ^ van Schaik, Carel; Deaner R, Merrill M (1999). "The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture". Journal of Human Evolution 36 (6): 719–741. doi:10.1006/jhev.1999.0304. PMID 10330335. http://main.gvsu.edu/cms3/assets/6D2549F6-ED41-142A-2D7251DEDEE796B4/deanerfiles/2vanSchaiketal1999PrimateMaterialCulture.pdf. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
15. ^ a b Cummins-Sebree, SE; Fragaszy DM (2005). Capuchins as Stone-knappers?: an Evaluation of the Evidence. Stone Knapping: The Necessary Conditions for a Uniquely Hominin Behaviour. Cambridge, England: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research. pp. 171. ISBN 9781902937342. 16. ^ Costello, M; D. Fragaszy (1988). "Prehension in Cebus and Saimiri: Grip type and hand preference". American Journal of Primatology 15: 235–245. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350150306. http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/110487513/abstract. Retrieved 2009-12.
17. ^ Rilling, James; Thomas R. Insel (August 1999). "The primate neocortex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging". Journal of Human Evolution 37 (2): 191–223. doi:10.1006/jhev.1999.0313. PMID 10444351. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0047248499903135. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
18. ^ Dunbar, Robin (1998). "The social brain hypothesis". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 6 (5): 178–190. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5<178::AID-EVAN5>3.0.CO;2-8. http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/38663/abstract?CRETRY=1&SRETRY=0. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
19. ^ Izar, Patricia (2004). "Female social relationships of Cebus apella nigritus in a southeastern Atlantic forest". Behaviour 141 (1): 71–99. doi:10.1163/156853904772746619. http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/brill/beh/2004/00000141/00000001/art00006. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
20. ^ Visalberghi, Elisabetta; D. Fragaszy (1995). "The behaviour of capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella, with novel food: the role of social context". Animal Behaviour 49 (4): 1089–1095. doi:10.1006/anbe.1995.0137. 2009-12-04.
21. ^ a b Westergaard, Gregory Charles; D. Fragaszy (1987). "The manufacture and use of tools by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology 101 (2): 159–168. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.101.2.159. http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=fulltext.journal&jcode=com&vol=101&issue=2&format=html&page=159&expand=1. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
22. ^ a b Moura, A.C.; Lee, PC (2004). "Capuchin stone tool use in Caatinga dry forest". Science 306 (5703): 1909. doi:10.1126/science.1102558. PMID 15591195. http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/abstract/sci;306/5703/1909. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
23. ^ a b c Mannu, Massimo; Eduardo Ottoni (2009). "The enhanced tool-kit of two groups of wild bearded capuchin monkeys in the Caatinga: tool making, associative use, and secondary tools". American Journal of Primatology 71 (3): 242–251. doi:10.1002/ajp.20642. PMID 19051323. http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/121542375/abstract. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
24. ^ Fragaszy, Dorothy; Patrícia Izar, Elisabetta Visalberghi, Eduardo B. Ottoni, Marino Gomes de Oliveira (2004). "Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools". American Journal of Primatology 64 (4): 359–366. doi:10.1002/ajp.20085. PMID 15580579. http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/109805939/abstract. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
25. ^ http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/abstract/306/5703/1909
26. ^ http://www.springerlink.com/content/m873402644714130/
27. ^ http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ajp.20085/abstract
28. ^ http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/308/5724/951c
29. ^ Visalberghi, Elisabetta; Trinca L (1989). "Tool use in capuchin monkeys: Distinguishing between performing and understanding". Primates 30 (4): 511–521. doi:10.1007/BF02380877. http://www.springerlink.com/content/h7230580177414v0/. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
30. ^ Fragaszy, Dorothy; Elisabetta Visalberghi (1989). "Social influences on the acquisition of tool-using behaviors in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology 103 (2): 159–170. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.103.2.159. PMID 2736909. http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=buy.optionToBuy&id=1989-31970-001&CFID=4966408&CFTOKEN=27125356. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
31. ^ a b Custance, D; Whiten A, Fredman T (1999). "Social learning of an artificial fruit task in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology 113: 13–22. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.113.1.13. http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/com/113/1/13.html. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
32. ^ Dindo, M; Thierry B, Whiten A (2008). "Social diffusion of novel foraging methods in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences 275 (1631): 187. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1318. PMID 17971322. PMC 2596191. http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/275/1631/187.full. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
33. ^ Dindo, M; Whiten A, De Waal F (2009). "In-Group Conformity Sustains Different Foraging Traditions in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella)". PloS one 4 (11): e7858. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007858. PMID 19924242. PMC 2773420. http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0007858. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
34. ^ Visalberghi, Elisabetta; Fragaszy DM (2002). " Do Monkeys Ape?"-Ten Years After. Imitation in animals and artifacts. MIT Press. pp. 494. ISBN 0262042037. 35. ^ Hunt, Gavin (1996). "Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows". Nature 379 (6562): 249–251. doi:10.1038/379249a0. http://www.nature.com/doifinder/10.1038/379249a0. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
36. ^ Cummins-Sebree, SE; D. Fragaszy (2005). "Choosing and using tools: Capuchins (Cebus apella) use a different metric than tamarins (Saguinus oedipus)". Journal of Comparative Psychology 119 (9): 210–219. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.210. PMID 15982164. http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=buy.optionToBuy&id=2005-06515-009&CFID=4966408&CFTOKEN=27125356. Retrieved 2009-12-04.
37. ^ a b Visalberghi, Elisabetta; Limognelli L. (1994). "Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology 108 (1): 511–521. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.108.1.15. PMID 8174341. http://psycnet.apa.org/?fa=main.doiLanding&doi=10.1037/0735-7036.108.1.15. Retrieved 2009-12-04.


* Notes on the taxonomy and distributions of the tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus, Cebidae) of South America
* Janson, Charles H. (2001) "Capuchin-like Monkeys". in: MacDonald, D. (red.), The New Encyclopedia of Mammals, Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 344-353.
* Nowak, R.M. (1999) Walker's Primates of the World. Baltimore & London: the Johns Hopkins University Press. p. 113-115.

Mammals Images

Biology Encyclopedia

Retrieved from "http://en.wikipedia.org/"
All text is available under the terms of the GNU Free Documentation License

Home - Hellenica World